“Ladder of Ascent and Descent of the Mind”.

 

 

Back and outward from humans
Organisms that we are familiar with—cats, rats and elephants—are related to one another on the tree of life, often pictured as a phylogeny. They are not on a ladder of life from simplest to most complex, not in a linear order. If we were to survey the diversity of organisms alive today on Earth, we couldn’t start at one end and march through from one to the next and then arrive at humans. Humans are not at the top, when it comes to phylogenetic relationships. No organism is at the top. And if we restrict ourselves to organisms living today, none is at the base either. We are all just cousins, more or less related to one another. When you think about it for a minute, it doesn’t make sense to call any organism ‘unrelated’ to us. Oak trees are related to us, they are just a lot less closely related to us than acorn woodpeckers.

Figure 1. Scala naturae as depicted by Ramon Llull (1305) in “Ladder of Ascent and Descent of the Mind”.

Aristotle, medieval philosophers, and naturalists a century before Darwin often tried to discern a linear scala naturae. Piecing together bits from Aristotle, it would go something like this: rocks, fungi, plants, worms meaning ‘invertebrates’, fish, whales and seals, reptiles, beasts on four legs, Man. But there’s something wrong with this linearization. Can you think of a reptile that has some fantastic special adaptations of its own? How about a whale? How about an invertebrate? And then there are plenty of plants and fungi that have amazing derived features. Just consider how whales sign songs to one another over long distances.

Books and courses in Canvas are typically arranged in a line. You learn things along the arrow of time. If you write something down for someone else, they will read it linearly. Therefore, we want to develop your knowledge of organismal diversity not as it actually is, rather from a much more biased perspective. We choose to teach you about life’s diversity from the perspective of humans. To do this, we make this perspective a retrospective. Stand as a human and look backwards in time. Look backward to the co-ancestor of humans with our closest cousins. Our closest living cousins are the chimpanzees, of which there are two species, the Common Chimp (Pan troglodytes) and the Bonobo (Pan paniscus). We are equally closely related to the Common Chimp and to the Bonobo. They are more closely related to each other than they are to us. We differ from them in a few ways—we speak languages that are infinitely recombinable, whereas their communications are much more limited; we stand upright in a precarious posture, whereas they are much sturdier knuckle walkers; we are sparsely hairy, whereas they are somewhat hairier. Nevertheless, we share a great deal in common with chimpanzees. This is because we and they all share a common ancestor who lived roughly 6 million years ago. From that common ancestor humans and chimps inherited a number of shared traits.

Now, look a bit further back in time to the confluence of our lineage with that of gorillas. Our (and chimps) most recent common ancestor with gorillas lived on the order of 8 million years ago (approximately). There are two species of gorilla, so the common ancestor of the African Apes gave rise to five species that are currently alive (two types of gorillas, two types of chimps, and all humans). The African Apes is the name of the clade that contains these five living species. It also contains some other species that are now extinct and that are poorly known from a few fossils. In undertaking our retrospective, let us ignore extinct forms, for the sake of simplicity. Only consider organisms that are alive today.

You can continue this tale of ancestors moving backwards in time from humans to our deep ancestors, and simultaneously moving outwards from humans to our more and more remote cousins. A good tool for looking up this information is

http://www.onezoom.org/ (Links to an external site.)

Start by searching for “humans” and then zoom out. Onezoom ignores extinct forms, which greatly simplifies the lesson that we are trying to have you learn. Go backwards from humans and at each confluence, write down (a) the clade that our lineages is merging with or some example in that clade, (b) the date of the confluence, i.e., the estimate of how many millions of years ago the common ancestor lived, (c) the name of the larger clade encompassing both branches it gave rise to if it is named, (d) the number of species in the larger clade. Write this on a long piece of paper or a Word document, starting at the top like so:

1-chimps 6 MYA unnamed 3 species
2-gorillas 8 MYA African Apes 5 species
3-etc.

Continue the ancestors’ tale back to and including the common ancestor with Fungi. How many confluences of still-living clades were there from humans to that point? In other words, how many lines of data are on your paper? Are you surprised at how large the number is, or how small, or is it just what you would have guessed? How many confluences of still-living clades were there from humans back to the common ancestor of all Vertebrates? How about to the common ancestor of all Primates?

Back and outward from some other organism
Now, do the same thing with some plant or animal other than a vertebrate. It could be an oak tree or a fruit fly or whatever you can find in onezoom. As you go take notes about the clades that are joining with your focal clade as you go. What are the distinctive features of co-ancestors? Please note: the tree-of-life that biologists have inferred is not completely resolved—not even close!—and onezoom is presenting a completely resolved, always bifurcating phylogeny. To do this, onezoom is guessing at one of various possible ways in which the phylogeny might have bifurcated. Be sure not to believe each phylogenetic detail, especially for organisms that are poorly studied.

What do you make of it?
After you have done this exercise and thought about it, you can write a paper. Some of your paper can concisely narrate the two ancestors’ tales. However, you should also explain what it means to you. In what sense do we object to the distinction between ‘higher (advanced) animals’ and ‘lower (primitive) animals’? How does the concept of the “ladder-of-life” or scala naturae fail? How about cartoons like the following?

 

 

Sample Solution

ty only detecting one letter, the T2, and in the single-detection task 85% of participants were able to correctly identify T2 in They also found that only 60% of participants could detect T2 in a dual-detection task between 180-450ms after T1 was presented. This is similar to our finding that T2 detection was worse when there was a dual-detection task and T2 was presented 300ms after T1. This consistency with results from previous studies indicates a high level of external reliability and supports the work by Raymond et al. (1992).

The findings in this study however, may be limited as they cannot offer conclusive evidence to support some of the existing theories explaining AB. For example, each trial in this study used RSVP with a stream of 12 letters. But to prove the interference theory proposed by Shaprio et al. (1997a) trials with a varying number of distractors may be needed to demonstrate that AB in due to competition between the targets and masks in the buffer. Similarly, to support the 2-stage model by Chun & Potter (1995) a modification to the study may be needed so it is possible to determine how much processing T2 undergoes. This could involve the use of EEG or priming techniques as seen in previous studies (Luck et al. 1996 ; Shapiro et al., 1997b) or alternatively the use of other neuroscience techniques such as fMRI to see if brain areas linked to processing meaning are active during the period when T2 is presented.

Furthermore, there are a number of questions left unanswered by both the present and existing studies on AB. For example, most existing theories suggest that the masks or distractors play a crucial role in the AB, but their exact role is not specified. A recent study by Nieuwenstein, Potter & Theeuwes (2010) suggests that the AB can occur without distractors. They conducted a RSVP experiment in which T1 and T2 were separated by a blank interval and the AB was still found to occur. These results could be interpreted to suggest that it our inability to rapidly reengage attention, shortly after disengaging attention following identification of T1 which leads to the AB, rather than the effect of distractors or depletion of processing resources. This has implications for existing theories and thus a new model for AB may be needed.

Another question which needs answering is why processing of T2 is relatively unaffected when it is presented immediately after T1. The work by Nieuwenstein et al. (2010) could also offer an explanation for this. Their work suggests target obdurate stimuli will elicit the deployment of attentional processing resources. Therefore, if T2 immediately follows T1 attentional engagement will be sustained, allowing T2 to be identified. If a distractor, or blank interval was to follow T1 then there will be a discontinuation of the input which kept attention engaged, and so attention will disengage prior to the presentation of T2 meaning it cannot be reported.

Overall, it is clear that while results found in this study are in line with others there is a lack of certainty in what causes these results, demonstrated by the number of contradicting and conflicting theories presented to in order explain AB. In the future further research into the AB could have a focus on, the role of distractors and timing of the AB in order to create a single unified model.

This question has been answered.

Get Answer
WeCreativez WhatsApp Support
Our customer support team is here to answer your questions. Ask us anything!
👋 Hi, Welcome to Compliant Papers.