Results in an order for IV antibiotics

 

A patient is ready for discharge when she spikes a fever of 101.3°F. A call to the physician results in an order for IV antibiotics to be administered every 12 hours for 48 hours. The patient’s family arrives to take her home, and they discover that she now has an IV and will not be discharged for 2 days. They ask, “What happened? Did our mother catch something in the hospital? We thought this is a place of healing.” How will you respond? Your response may have legal implications.
In one well-developed paragraphs (12-point font, min 7 sentences):
Describe one strategy you will incorporate in your practice to ensure that you are providing evidence-based care in the prevention of HAIs.

 

 

Sample Solution

One strategy I will incorporate in my practice to ensure that I am providing evidence-based care in the prevention of healthcare-associated infections (HAIs) is always utilizing personal protective equipment (PPE). This includes wearing masks, gowns, and gloves when interacting with patients and cleaning up after them. PPE is effective at minimizing direct contact with potential infection sources and helps protect both healthcare workers and patients from acquiring an HAI. Additionally, I will routinely wash my hands before and after patient encounters using soap or an alcohol-based hand rub for a minimum of 20 seconds each time. Washing hands regularly is essential to reducing the spread of germs between patients and healthcare workers, as well as preventing contamination of medical supplies or environmental surfaces. Finally, proper environmental cleaning protocols must also be followed to reduce risk of infection transmission through hard surfaces. This includes wiping down all frequently touched items like countertops, door handles, bedsides tables etc., with hospital grade disinfectant wipes while paying special attention to areas where bodily fluids may come into contact such as bathrooms or operating rooms. Utilizing these strategies can significantly lower risk of transmission within healthcare settings and help protect both staff members and patients from acquiring potentially dangerous HAIs.(McNeil 2016)

Range of outright judgment

The range of outright judgment is characterized as the cutoff to the precision with which one can distinguish the extent of a unidimensional boost variable (Miller, 1956), with this breaking point or length generally being around 7 + 2. Mill operator refers to Hayes memory range explore as proof for his restricting range. In this members needed to review data read out loud to them and results plainly showed that there was a typical furthest constraint of 9 when paired things were utilized. This was regardless of the consistent data speculation, which has recommended that the range ought to be long if each introduced thing contained little data (Miller, 1956). The end from Hayes and Pollack’s trials (see figure 1) was that how much data communicated expansions in a straight style alongside how much data per unit input (Miller, 1956). Figure 1. Estimations of memory for data wellsprings of various sorts and digit remainders, contrasted with anticipated results for consistent data. Results from Hayes (left) and Pollack (right) refered to by (Miller, 1956)

 

Pieces and lumps

Mill operator alludes to a ‘cycle’ of data as the need might have arisen ‘to go with a choice between two similarly logical other options’. Hence a basic either or choice requires the slightest bit of data; with more expected for additional complicated choices, along a twofold pathway (Miller, 1956). Decimal digits are worth 3.3 pieces each, implying that a 7-digit telephone number (what is effectively recalled) would include 23 pieces of data. Anyway an evident inconsistency to this is the way that, assuming an English word is worth around 10 pieces and just 23 pieces could be recalled then just 2-3 words could be recollected at any one time, clearly wrong. The restricting range can more readily be grasped with regards to the osmosis of pieces into lumps. Mill operator recognizes pieces and lumps of data, the differentiation being that a piece is comprised of various pieces of data. It is fascinating to take note of that while there is a limited ability to recall lumps of data, how much pieces in every one of those lumps can change broadly (Miller, 1956). Anyway it’s anything but a straightforward instance of having the memorable option huge pieces right away, somewhat that as each piece turns out to be more natural, it very well may be acclimatized into a lump, which is then recollected itself. Recoding is the interaction by which individual pieces are ‘recoded’ and allocated to lumps.

Consequently the ends that can be drawn from Miller’s unique work is that, while there is an acknowledged breaking point to the quantity of pieces of data that can be put away in prompt (present moment) memory, how much data inside every one of those lumps can be very high, without unfavorably influencing the review of similar number of lumps. The cutting edge perspective on momentary memory limit Millers sorcery number 7+2 has been all the more as of late reclassified to the enchanted number 4+1 (Cowan, 2001). The test has come from results, for example, those from Chen and Cowan, in which the anticipated outcomes from a trial were that prompt sequential review of outright quantities of singleton words would be equivalent to the quantity of pieces of learned pair words. Anyway truth be told it was found that a similar number of pre-uncovered singleton words was reviewed as the quantity of words inside educated matches – eg 8 words (introduced as 8 singletons or 4 learned sets). Anyway 6 learned matches could be reviewed as effectively as 6 pre-uncovered singleton words (Chen and Cowan, 2005). This recommended an alternate system for review contingent upon the conditions. Cowan alludes to the greatest number of lumps that can be reviewed as the memory stockpiling limit (Cowan, 2001). It is noticed that the quantity of pieces can be impacted by long haul memory data, as demonstrated by Miller regarding recoding – with extra data to empower this recoding coming from long haul memory.

 

Factors influencing clear transient memory

Practice

The penchant to utilize practice and memory helps is a serious complexity in precisely estimating the limit of transient memory. To be sure a significant number of the investigations pompously estimating momentary memory limit have been contended to be really estimating the capacity to practice and access long haul memory stores (Cowan, 2001). Considering that recoding includes practice and the utilization of long haul memory arrangement, whatever forestalls or impacts these will clearly influence the capacity to recode effectively (Cowan, 2001).

This question has been answered.

Get Answer
WeCreativez WhatsApp Support
Our customer support team is here to answer your questions. Ask us anything!
👋 Hi, Welcome to Compliant Papers.